• Skip to main content
  • Skip to secondary menu
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer

Life in the Fast Lane • LITFL • Medical Blog

Emergency medicine and critical care medical education blog

  • MENU
  • ECG
  • CCC
  • Top 100
  • PODCASTS
  • JOBS
  • TOX
  • PART ONE
  • INTENSIVE
Home | LITFL | Clinical Cases | Are you Satisficed?

Are you Satisficed?

by Dr Chris Nickson, last update March 21, 2019

aka To Err is human 002

At handover in ICU, a junior resident mentions that an intubated and ventilated 68 year-old man, who had cardiac surgery 4 days previously, has developed an increasing FiO2 requirement over the course of the day. He says this is explained by a new left lower zone opacity that was seen on the patient’s chest radiograph in the morning. Antibiotics were started for a hospital acquired pneumonia.

After handover is finished you review the patient’s chest x-ray taken in the morning:

to err is human cxr 001 pthx
The patient’s most recent CXR

..and compare it to his previous chest film, taken the day before:

to err is human cxr 002 pthx
The patient’s previous CXR

Questions

Q1. What are the chest radiograph findings?

Answer and interpretation

Your colleague was correct, there is a new left lower zone opacity. This could represent consolidation, collapse and/or an effusion. This finding would contribute to an increased FiO2 requirement.

Numerous other chest radiograph findings are unchanged — they include:

  • sternal wires
  • cardiomegaly
  • bilateral alveolar opacities and prominent upper lobe vessels consistent with pulmonary edema
  • a replaced mitral valve
  • various equipment:
    a correctly positioned endotracheal tube, a pulmonary artery catheter, an internal jugular vein central venous catheter, pacing wires and a tangle of other monitoring leads.

However, there is also one important new finding: the development of a right-sided pneumothorax. Most likely this has enlarged over the course of the day as a result of positive pressure ventilation, and is probably a major factor contributing to the patient’s increasing oxygen requirements.


Q2. What cognitive bias or heuristic can lead to abnormalities being missed in a situation like this?

Answer and interpretation

This heuristic (a cognitive “rule of thumb” or short-cut) causes us to stop searching for further findings once one abnormality has been found. It is a universal human tendency. The term ‘satisficing‘ was coined by Nobel prize-winning economist and polymath Herbert Simon and is a hybrid of ‘satisfying’ and ‘sufficing’.

In the absence of the left lower zone opacity the resident probably would have searched harder for another cause of the increasing oxygen requirement. By finding an obvious abnormality that potentially explained the deterioration his ‘search satisficing’ needs were fulfilled.

‘Search satisficing’ is a type of premature closure, which describes the more general tendency to end a decision-making process early, before the decision has been completely verified.

‘When the diagnosis is made, the thinking stops’.


Q3. What are some other situations where this cognitive bias or heuristic may be important?

Answer and interpretation

The possibilities are nearly endless in medicine.

Some classic examples include:

  • Co-ingestants in drug overdoses
    — the unconscious patient brought in with an empty pack of diazepam — maybe the paramedics didn’t find the the empty pack of amitriptyline under the bed?
    [Check out this Poison Review example of premature closure almost leading to trouble]
  • Removing foreign bodies from wounds
    — the euphoria of finally extracting a hard-won foreign body can easily distract from the need to search for more.
  • Trauma patients with spine injuries
    — there may be multiple injuries at multiple levels, particularly when it comes to C-spine fractures.
  • Fall from a height with heel pain
    — the calcaneal fracture may be obvious, but what about the associated pelvis or spine fractures?
  • Injuries to ring structures
    — injuries to so-called ‘ring structures’ (such as the pelvis, mandible or ankle) typically fracture in more than one place.

Leave a comment if you can think of other important situations where ‘search satisficing’ may lead to errors.


Q4. How can this cognitive bias or heuristic be avoided?

Answer and interpretation

Some ways to avoid making errors due to ‘search satisficing’ are to:

  • Use metacognition (‘think about thinking’)
    — be aware of our universal tendency to ‘search satisfice’ and know when it is likely to be important.
  • Be systematic in the situations when it is likely to occur
    — don’t rush or take short-cuts. That’s one reason why ‘tertiary surveys’ are so important in trauma patients.
  • Use checklists
    — an example is the standardised trauma sheet used in many hospitals.
    — make the statement, “there is, or is not, a pneumothorax present”, a routine part of CXR interpretation.
  • Ask yourself: “Could there be something else going on?”
    — this is especially important if the patient continues to deteriorate despite treatment.
  • ‘Phone a friend’
    — get a second opinion if available, doctors always like to find stuff that their colleagues have overlooked!
    [see the role of the consultant]

Q5. Why does this cognitive bias or heuristic exist?

Answer and interpretation

In general cognitive biases and heuristics probably exist because they conferred an evolutionary advantage to our ancestors (error management theory), or because of ‘bounded rationality‘.

From the error management theory perspective, ‘search satisficing’ may have been a useful survival strategy for hunter-gathers, in that ‘a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush’. According to the theory of bounded rationality, our ability to reason is limited by:

  1. the information available,
  2. our cognitive abilities, and
  3. the limited time we have to make decisions.In situations where a fully optimized reasoning process is simply not feasible due to the above limitations, we use heuristics instead. Such heuristics are useful because they work most of the time… but it is inevitable that they will sometimes lead to mistakes.

References
  • Weingart S, Wyer P. Emergency Medicine Decision Making: Critical Choices in Chaotic Environments. McGraw-Hill
  • Crosskerry P, Cosby KS, Schenkel SM, Wears RL. Patient Safety in Emergency Medicine. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

to err is human 700

CLINICAL CASES

to err is human

more cases…

Share this:

  • Facebook
  • Twitter
  • LinkedIn
  • Print

Related

About Dr Chris Nickson

An oslerphile emergency physician and intensivist suffering from a bad case of knowledge dipsosis. Key areas of interest include: the ED-ICU interface, toxicology, simulation and the free open-access meducation (FOAM) revolution. @Twitter | INTENSIVE| SMACC

Reader Interactions

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

Primary Sidebar

  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • Vimeo
  • LinkedIn
  • Twitter
  • Twitter
  • RSS Feed

Recent Posts

Adult CXR Cases 002

Is climate crisis a medical emergency?

Pain scale

Pediatric CXR Cases 007

Name that murmur

Footer

RSS FEED  LITFL posts by EMAIL or RSS

RSS FEED  LITFL Review by EMAIL or RSS

RSS FEED  FFFF by EMAIL or RSS

#FOAMed Medical Education Resources by LITFL is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.

Based on a work at https://litfl.com

  • Twitter
  • Vimeo
  • Facebook
  • LinkedIn
  • Twitter
  • Twitter

Authors • Blog • Contact • Disclaimer

2018 Launch

  • 22,445,315 visitors

Copyright © 2019 · Powered by vocortex and iSimulate